No totes les abelles tenen el mateix caràcter, existint
diversitat de comportaments, des del més atrevit fins al més conservador. L’estudi
va utilitzar poblacions d’abelles, i amb marcatges toràcics van poder valorar
com en la gran explosió demogràfica apareixien escamots d’abelles especialment
curioses. Aquestes exploradores serien molt interessants en processos d’eixamenada,
quan la recerca d’una nova llar i de noves fonts d’aliment són qüestió de vida
o mort.
Some bees are born curious while others are more
single-minded – new research hints at how the hive picks which flowers to feast
on
When you try to pick a restaurant with a group of friends,
how do you decide? Your curious friend wants to try the new place, while your
focused friend wants to go to the old faithful. One friend is insistent, while
the other is more quiet. Ultimately, the focused vocal friend convinces the
group by saying, “I am telling you, this is the best place. It’s a sure thing –
we gotta go!”
Just like people, honey bees vary in how they seek out food
and communicate where to go. As a biologist, I study collective behavior,
especially how groups make decisions. My colleagues and I have discovered some
individual bees are seemingly born with a predetermined foraging style – they
can be either focused foragers or curious foragers. Having different approaches
to collecting food turns out to be advantageous for large colonies that rely on
a changing food landscape.
Explorers and exploiters
As animals collect food, they must balance exploring for new
food with exploiting already known food sources. Individual animals have to do
one or the other, switching between exploring or exploiting. In collectives,
like honey bee colonies, foragers can split the work and do both at the same
time.
As honey bees forage for nectar and pollen, they learn a lot
of information about the flowers they visit, such as their smells, colors and
locations. Some bees become extremely focused on information associated with
food, ignoring any new information – similar to selective attention in humans.
Conversely, other bees exhibit a learning behavior marked by curiosity. They
are interested in learning about new food sources, not just familiar ones.
True to type
My colleagues and I became interested in how bee colonies
manage and act on these two types of information. To answer this question, we
first figured out how to breed curious bees and focused bees.
We tested female queens and male drones to see if they were
curious or focused, and then used artificial insemination to breed a curious
queen with a curious drone, and a focused queen with a focused drone. Typically
queens mate with 12 to 15 different drones and create genetically diverse
workers, so using a single drone helped keep workers genetically uniform.
A bowl filled with several hundred bees, all marked with a
blue dot on the thorax.
One-day-old curious bees marked blue. Sebastian Scofield, CC
BY-SA
Once we had populations of genetically curious and focused
bees, we had to verify they would not be influenced by their social
environment. We did this by placing bees in colonies of either their own
learning type or one with an assortment of learning types. (We kept track of
who was who by marking them with paint on their thorax as soon as they were
born.) Sure enough, regardless of the social group the bees experienced, they
exhibited the same learning behavior we observed in their parents.
Familiar food versus novel food
Next, we created colonies of all focused bees, all curious
bees or a 50/50 mix of focused and curious bees – then watched how they
foraged.
We gave them a choice between two food locations: a
familiar, reliable food location that stayed in the same spot for four days or
a new food location that changed odor, color and location every day. Both
locations contained the same quality and quantity of food. We marked bees on
their abdomens as they visited the feeders so we knew which ones they had been
to and which ones they were revisiting.
Five bees perched on the edge of a red feeder, sipping
nectar.
Researchers marked the bees visiting this feeder with
yellow. Chelsea Cook, CC BY-SA
We discovered the focused colony quickly found the familiar
food location and exploited that eatery all week, rarely visiting the novel
food option.
The curious colony, as expected, visited the novel and the
familiar food locations equally, showing no preference.
Interestingly, the 50/50 mixed colony ended up acting more
like the focused colony, using the familiar feeder and paying little attention
to the novel feeders. We observed the curious bees in the mixed colony shifted
their selected behavior by visiting the familiar feeder more than the novel
one. Why?
The bee in the middle communicates the location of food
using the waggle dance.
Dancing up a storm
When honey bees find a good source of food, they use the
waggle dance to direct their nest mates. This dance communicates the distance
to and direction of a nutritious meal, as well as its perceived quality. When
we looked at waggle dance behavior in the 50/50 colony, we saw the focused bees
were dancing more intensely – performing 0.59 turns per second, significantly
faster than the curious bees’ 0.52 turns per second. Just like your vocal,
excited friend, the focused bees attracted more followers, so more bees were
recruited to the familiar, reliable feeder.
Because curious bees are interested in everything, including
new information about possible food locations, they are perfect listeners and
are easily convinced to visit the chosen feeder of their enthusiastic nest
mates.
Our future work will investigate how these foraging dynamics
work in a changing food landscape – one where food runs out. If a source is
depleted, will the focused bees turn their attention to the curious bees, who
already know where other foraging locations are?
This research suggests successful societies make better decisions
when members, by virtue of their innate learning styles, collect and
communicate a diversity of information – whether they are bees looking for
nectar or friends trying to decide on a restaurant. Diversity of learning
behavior in individuals may help social groups adapt to shifting global
environments.
theconversation.com