Inside the Wonderful World of Bee Cognition - Where We're at Now
By Felicity Muth
As I wrote about in my
last post, bees are capable of learning which
flowers offer good nectar rewards based on floral features such as colour,
smell, shape, texture, pattern, temperature and electric charge. They do this
through associative learning: learning that a ‘conditioned stimulus’ (for
example, the colour yellow) is associated with an ‘unconditioned stimulus’
(nectar). Learning simple associations like these is the basis of all learning
– pretty much all animals do it, from humans to the sea slug which doesn’t even
have a brain.
However, the world is
rarely as simple as this and so animals need to be flexible. For example, as
humans we might learn that if we put our bank card in a machine and enter a pin
number we can obtain money. However, we might also have to learn that we can
only access the bank machine inside the bank during particular hours, or that
if we travel to another country their bank machines might operate differently.
Therefore we need some behavioural flexibility around what we’ve learned. The
same is true for bees. In a bee’s world, much of what she learns relates to
getting food from flowers. However, it won’t always be as simple as ‘blue
flowers have better nectar than yellow rewards’. Instead a bee might have to
learn ‘blue flowers have better nectar than yellow flowers, but only in the
morning’ or ‘this particular species of blue flower which also has a specific
smell has better nectar than yellow flowers, but another species of blue flower
has worse nectar’.
Honeybees can indeed
learn more complex relationships like this. This has been shown in many
different experiments using different protocols and in different contexts. For
example, bees can be trained that an artificial flower which has a blue
checkered pattern has good nectar rewards, and one with a yellow checkered
pattern has good nectar rewards but a combination of the two (blue and yellow
checkered) is not good. They can also be trained to the reverse (that the
combination of the two stimuli is good, but that either by themselves is not
good). Similarly, honeybees can be trained that only very particular
combinations of stimuli are good; i.e. A and B together are good, and C and D
together are good, but any other combination (e.g. A and C or B and D) are not
good. The list of other complex relationships bees can learn is seemingly
endless, but other impressive feats include honeybees’ ability to learn that
rewards can be found in a specific location only at one particular time of day
and that bumblebees can learn that the location of nectar alternates between two available options and solve physical problems.
However, honeybees’
and bumblebees’ cognitive abilities go beyond these examples of simply learning
about their worlds, be it under a number of complex conditions. One excellent
study showed that bees could actually form abstract concepts about their world.
Having an abstract concept is the ability to understand a general fact about
the way things are and to being able to generalise that fact to new situations
you might encounter, as opposed to learning relationships that only hold in one
particular situation. As humans, we form abstract concepts about the world all
the time, generalising from one situation to another. For example, one concept
we form about the world is the concept of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’. If we
were having dinner together and I asked you if you’d like ‘more of the same’,
you would understand that if we had just been eating pasta that I was offering
you more pasta. In another, totally different situation, say we’re operating on
someone together and I ask you to pass me ‘the same instrument for stitching
people closed that you just gave me a minute ago’ (I’m not sure why any doctor
would ever phrase it this way; but let’s just suppose that they don’t have a
great memory for medical instrument names), you would understand that you
needed to pass me another needle. Therefore, you have the ability to take the
concept of ‘sameness’ and use it in two totally different situations. But how
would you go about asking a bee if she can do the same thing?
Researchers did this
through a cleverly thought-out experiment. First they trained a bee that if she
saw a particular colour (say, blue) then when she was later given a choice
between blue and yellow, blue always had nectar whereas yellow did not (stages
1 and 2 on the diagram). Similarly, she was trained that if she saw yellow then
when she was later given a choice, she had to choose yellow to get the reward
(steps 3 and 4 on the diagram). Therefore, she always had to go to the same
colour as the one she had previously seen to get the reward. The bees learned
this without much difficulty. However, at this point it’s not clear whether the
bee had actually learned the concept of ‘sameness’ or instead had just learned
a rule for this one situation (e.g. ‘I go to yellow to get a reward when I see
yellow and I go to blue to get a reward when I see blue’). To test whether the
bees had actually learned the concept of ‘same’, the researchers then presented
the bee with a new stimulus, one she had never seen before. This time it was a
pattern: black and white horizontal stripes. The bee was then given a ‘transfer
test’; a choice between a black and white striped horizontal pattern or a
vertical pattern. If the bee had learned the rule ‘when I see a stimulus I then
need to choose the same stimulus to get a reward’ (i.e. the concept of ‘same’)
then she should fly to the horizontal stripes pattern (steps 5 and 6 on the
diagram). This is indeed what the majority of bees did. Another group of bees
were trained only to black and white horizontal patterns and then given
transfer tests using blue and yellow colours; these bees also showed that they
had learned the concept of ‘same’ by going to the correct colour. Now, the
really cool part of this experiment was that the researchers then gave a new
set of bees stimuli in a totally different modality: scent. Bees were trained
that when they smelled a particular odour, they had to go to the same odour to
get a reward. They were then given a transfer test in colour, and the bees
transferred their knowledge to this new context, going to the ‘correct’ colour
even though they had never been trained with colour before. In another set of
bees, individuals were trained to go to the different stimulus to the one they
had just seen before being given a transfer test, and their choices showed that
they were also able to learn the concept of ‘difference’.
After I tell people
about some of these impressive cognitive abilities that bees have, another
question that I often get asked is, ‘OK, so if bees are so smart, then why do
they always fly into windows?’. I hope from what you’ve read in these two posts
you can appreciate that when you want to ask a question of a bee you have to
frame it in a way that the bee ‘understands’. If we were to ask a human a
question, we could use language, to ask a bee a question, you generally use
stimuli that represent flowers and nectar. Like all animals, the cognitive
abilities of bees have been selected by natural selection to make the bee as
good as possible at learning about things that it needs to know about its
environment. This includes many complex relationships about how to get the best
food from flowers, but sadly, doesn’t include the ability of how to best
navigate windows.
Foto: jinterwas
0 comments:
Publica un comentari a l'entrada